A growing body of evidence suggests that adults can monitor other people’s beliefs in an efficient way. However, the nature and the limits of efficient belief tracking are still being debated. The present study addressed these issues by testing (a) whether adults spontaneously process other people’s beliefs when overt task instructions assign priority to participants’ own belief, (b) whether this processing relies on low-level associative processes and (c) whether the propensity to track other people’s beliefs is linked to empathic disposition. Adult participants were asked to alternately judge an agent’s belief and their own belief. These beliefs were either consistent or inconsistent with each other. Furthermore, visual association betwee...
The lack of consensus on how to characterize humans' capacity for belief reasoning has been brought ...
This study investigated the processing of beliefs, i.e. the computation we do to represent what some...
This study explored how efficiently younger (18-30 years) and older (65-80 years) adults compute bel...
The main question of Theory of Mind research is not only how we represent others’ mental states, but...
While adults can readily report another agent’s false belief, theories of belief processing typicall...
Several theories of belief processing assume that processing another’s false belief requires overcom...
While adults can readily report another agent’s false belief, theories of belief processing typicall...
Three experiments investigated efficient belief tracking as described by the two-systems theory of h...
Recent findings suggest that tracking others' beliefs is not always effortful and slow, but may rely...
Characterizing the cognitive architecture of human mindreading forces us to address two puzzles in p...
ABSTRACT—Understanding the operating characteristics of theory of mind is essential for understandin...
Five experiments investigated evidence for a dual-process account of mindreading (Apperly, 2010). Th...
In social situations, adults ascribe mental states to other people and to themselves. They contrast ...
‘Theory of Mind’ refers to the ability to attribute mental states to oneself and other people (Prema...
The ability to attribute and represent others’ mental states (e.g., beliefs; so-called ‘‘theory of m...
The lack of consensus on how to characterize humans' capacity for belief reasoning has been brought ...
This study investigated the processing of beliefs, i.e. the computation we do to represent what some...
This study explored how efficiently younger (18-30 years) and older (65-80 years) adults compute bel...
The main question of Theory of Mind research is not only how we represent others’ mental states, but...
While adults can readily report another agent’s false belief, theories of belief processing typicall...
Several theories of belief processing assume that processing another’s false belief requires overcom...
While adults can readily report another agent’s false belief, theories of belief processing typicall...
Three experiments investigated efficient belief tracking as described by the two-systems theory of h...
Recent findings suggest that tracking others' beliefs is not always effortful and slow, but may rely...
Characterizing the cognitive architecture of human mindreading forces us to address two puzzles in p...
ABSTRACT—Understanding the operating characteristics of theory of mind is essential for understandin...
Five experiments investigated evidence for a dual-process account of mindreading (Apperly, 2010). Th...
In social situations, adults ascribe mental states to other people and to themselves. They contrast ...
‘Theory of Mind’ refers to the ability to attribute mental states to oneself and other people (Prema...
The ability to attribute and represent others’ mental states (e.g., beliefs; so-called ‘‘theory of m...
The lack of consensus on how to characterize humans' capacity for belief reasoning has been brought ...
This study investigated the processing of beliefs, i.e. the computation we do to represent what some...
This study explored how efficiently younger (18-30 years) and older (65-80 years) adults compute bel...