Notwithstanding alternative ways of obtaining power, social power is mostly commonly acquired through either a dominance approach, where power is grabbed by the powerholder, or a prestige approach, where power is granted by group members. Although children’s attitude toward power in the dominance situation has been studied, little is known about how children understand and distinguish different ways of obtaining power. We examined the understanding of power in children aged 4–8 years by their resource allocation behavior in two social power acquisition situations. In Study 1, 4- to 8-year-olds (N = 123) gradually shifted from distributing more to the powerholder to showing no preference for either party (in the prestige situation) or to dis...
Previous work has provided evidence that both merit and social relationships guide resource distribu...
This study investigated children’s evaluations of peer group members who deviated from group norms a...
In three experiments, we investigated whether 2- and 3-year-olds (N = 240) consider ownership when t...
Notwithstanding alternative ways of obtaining power, social power is mostly commonly acquired throug...
International audienceAbstractTwo experiments with preschoolers (36 to 78 months) and 8-year-old chi...
Two experiments with preschoolers (36 to 78 months) and 8-year-old children (Experiment 1, N=173, Ex...
Power differences organize social relations across species. They emerge early in development, and ar...
International audienceAbstractHierarchy is a recurrent feature of social life. From an early age, ch...
Past research reveals a tension between children's preferences for egalitarianism and ingroup favori...
Peer Reviewedhttps://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/137499/1/cdev12643.pdfhttps://deepblue...
Behaviour benefitting others (prosocial behaviour) can be motivated by self-interested strategic con...
Recent research has found that even preschoolers give more resources to others who have previously g...
Social dominance in early childhood has traditionally been viewed as a social status based on aggres...
© 2015 Elsevier Inc.Recent research on distributive justice suggests that young children prefer equa...
Through the lens of resource control and resource holding potential theory, an investigation was con...
Previous work has provided evidence that both merit and social relationships guide resource distribu...
This study investigated children’s evaluations of peer group members who deviated from group norms a...
In three experiments, we investigated whether 2- and 3-year-olds (N = 240) consider ownership when t...
Notwithstanding alternative ways of obtaining power, social power is mostly commonly acquired throug...
International audienceAbstractTwo experiments with preschoolers (36 to 78 months) and 8-year-old chi...
Two experiments with preschoolers (36 to 78 months) and 8-year-old children (Experiment 1, N=173, Ex...
Power differences organize social relations across species. They emerge early in development, and ar...
International audienceAbstractHierarchy is a recurrent feature of social life. From an early age, ch...
Past research reveals a tension between children's preferences for egalitarianism and ingroup favori...
Peer Reviewedhttps://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/137499/1/cdev12643.pdfhttps://deepblue...
Behaviour benefitting others (prosocial behaviour) can be motivated by self-interested strategic con...
Recent research has found that even preschoolers give more resources to others who have previously g...
Social dominance in early childhood has traditionally been viewed as a social status based on aggres...
© 2015 Elsevier Inc.Recent research on distributive justice suggests that young children prefer equa...
Through the lens of resource control and resource holding potential theory, an investigation was con...
Previous work has provided evidence that both merit and social relationships guide resource distribu...
This study investigated children’s evaluations of peer group members who deviated from group norms a...
In three experiments, we investigated whether 2- and 3-year-olds (N = 240) consider ownership when t...