It was hypothesized that 6-month-old infants may be able to use indirect landmarks to locate a goal if (a) the landmarks are sufficiently distinctive and (b) the goal location is between landmarks, rather than on the opposite side of the space as was used in earlier research. Six-month-old infants were tested in a peekaboo paradigm in which they had to turn to a target location after displacement to a novel position. Infants looked to the goal location significantly more in a beacon and an indirect landmarks condition relative to a control and a single landmark condition. These results are discussed in terms of current theories of spatial development
It has been suggested that learning an object's location relative to (1) intramaze landmarks and (2)...
Nine month old infants search correctly for an object which they have seen hidden in one position, b...
Three experiments are described that investigate 4.5-month-old infants' spatial thinking during pass...
The ability to use the relations between visible landmarks to locate nonvisible goals (allocentric s...
The overall aim of this research was to establish the youngest age at which use of beacons to aid sp...
Recent research has indicated that, particularly under conditions of inertial disorientation, mammal...
In order to locate objects in an enclosed environment animals and humans use visual and non-visual d...
In order to locate objects in an enclosed environment animals and humans use visual and non-visual d...
From a developmental perspective, it has been reasoned that, over the course of development, childre...
Movement perception facilitates spatial orienting of attention in infants (Farroni, Johnson, Brockba...
In spatial development representations of the environment and the use of spatial cues change over ti...
Adults learning to navigate to a hidden goal within an enclosed space have been found to prefer info...
ABSTRACT: Several theories of spatial orientation propose that the geometry of an environment plays ...
Several theories of spatial orientation propose that the geometry of an environment plays a privileg...
It has been suggested that learning an object's location relative to (1) intramaze landmarks and (2)...
It has been suggested that learning an object's location relative to (1) intramaze landmarks and (2)...
Nine month old infants search correctly for an object which they have seen hidden in one position, b...
Three experiments are described that investigate 4.5-month-old infants' spatial thinking during pass...
The ability to use the relations between visible landmarks to locate nonvisible goals (allocentric s...
The overall aim of this research was to establish the youngest age at which use of beacons to aid sp...
Recent research has indicated that, particularly under conditions of inertial disorientation, mammal...
In order to locate objects in an enclosed environment animals and humans use visual and non-visual d...
In order to locate objects in an enclosed environment animals and humans use visual and non-visual d...
From a developmental perspective, it has been reasoned that, over the course of development, childre...
Movement perception facilitates spatial orienting of attention in infants (Farroni, Johnson, Brockba...
In spatial development representations of the environment and the use of spatial cues change over ti...
Adults learning to navigate to a hidden goal within an enclosed space have been found to prefer info...
ABSTRACT: Several theories of spatial orientation propose that the geometry of an environment plays ...
Several theories of spatial orientation propose that the geometry of an environment plays a privileg...
It has been suggested that learning an object's location relative to (1) intramaze landmarks and (2)...
It has been suggested that learning an object's location relative to (1) intramaze landmarks and (2)...
Nine month old infants search correctly for an object which they have seen hidden in one position, b...
Three experiments are described that investigate 4.5-month-old infants' spatial thinking during pass...