This study investigated whether children learn from exploration and act as effective informants by providing informative demonstrations tailored to observers’ goals and competence. Children (4.0–6.9 years, N = 98) explored a causally ambiguous toy to discover its causal structure and then demonstrated the toy to a naive observer. Children provided more costly and informative evidence when the observer wanted to learn about the toy than observe its effects (Experiment 1) and when the observer was ordinary than exceptionally intelligent (Experiment 2). Relative to the evidence they generated during exploration, children produced fewer, less costly actions when the observer wanted or needed less evidence. Children understand the difference bet...
In five experiments, we examined 3- to 6-year-olds’ understanding that they could gain knowledge ind...
In three experiments, children aged between 3 and 5 years (N= 38, 52, 94; mean ages 3–7 to 5–2) indi...
The study is an eyetracking experiment run with 12- and 28-month old children and adults. Participan...
∗ The first two authors contributed equally to this work. Motivated by computational analyses, we lo...
We investigated whether children preferentially select informative actions and make accurate inferen...
This article explores ways of characterizing different dimensions and levels of scientific reasoning...
Much of what children learn is socially transmitted; comes from the explanations others provide, rat...
How does early social experience affect children's inferences and exploration? Following prior work ...
From early in life, young children learn conventional information, including artifact uses and forma...
Children often learn from others’ demonstrations, but in the causal domain evidence acquired from ob...
Motivated by computational analyses, we look at how teaching affects exploration and discovery. In E...
Children are judicious social learners. They may be particularly sensitive to communicative actions ...
Human beings are born curious and explorative by nature. Like little scientists, children, even inf...
New competencies may be learned through active experience (experiential learning or learning by doin...
In three experiments children aged between 3 and 5 years (N = 38; 52; 94; mean ages 3;7 to 5;2) indi...
In five experiments, we examined 3- to 6-year-olds’ understanding that they could gain knowledge ind...
In three experiments, children aged between 3 and 5 years (N= 38, 52, 94; mean ages 3–7 to 5–2) indi...
The study is an eyetracking experiment run with 12- and 28-month old children and adults. Participan...
∗ The first two authors contributed equally to this work. Motivated by computational analyses, we lo...
We investigated whether children preferentially select informative actions and make accurate inferen...
This article explores ways of characterizing different dimensions and levels of scientific reasoning...
Much of what children learn is socially transmitted; comes from the explanations others provide, rat...
How does early social experience affect children's inferences and exploration? Following prior work ...
From early in life, young children learn conventional information, including artifact uses and forma...
Children often learn from others’ demonstrations, but in the causal domain evidence acquired from ob...
Motivated by computational analyses, we look at how teaching affects exploration and discovery. In E...
Children are judicious social learners. They may be particularly sensitive to communicative actions ...
Human beings are born curious and explorative by nature. Like little scientists, children, even inf...
New competencies may be learned through active experience (experiential learning or learning by doin...
In three experiments children aged between 3 and 5 years (N = 38; 52; 94; mean ages 3;7 to 5;2) indi...
In five experiments, we examined 3- to 6-year-olds’ understanding that they could gain knowledge ind...
In three experiments, children aged between 3 and 5 years (N= 38, 52, 94; mean ages 3–7 to 5–2) indi...
The study is an eyetracking experiment run with 12- and 28-month old children and adults. Participan...