Contains fulltext : 209062.pdf (Publisher’s version ) (Closed access)Interference in picture naming occurs from representing a partner's preparations to speak (Gambi, van de Cavey, & Pickering, 2015). We tested the origins of this interference using a simple non-communicative joint naming task based on Gambi et al. (2015), where response latencies indexed interference from partner task and partner speech content, and eye fixations to partner objects indexed overt attention. Experiment 1 contrasted a partner-present condition with a control partner-absent condition to establish the role of the partner in eliciting interference. For latencies, we observed interference from the partner's task and speech content, with interfer...
Contains fulltext : 99699.pdf (publisher's version ) (Closed access) ...
Co-acting participants represent and integrate each other's actions, even when they are not required...
The understanding of the relationship between attention and normal language processing can provide i...
Interference in picture naming occurs from representing a partner's preparations to speak (Gambi, va...
In 4 experiments we showed that picture naming latencies are affected by beliefs about the task conc...
Contains fulltext : 143241pre.pdf (preprint version ) (Open Access)Previous dual-t...
Contains fulltext : 140314pre.pdf (preprint version ) (Open Access)Evidence from d...
Previous dual-task studies examining the locus of semantic interference of distractor words in pictu...
Disagreement exists regarding the functional locus of semantic interference of distractor words in p...
Evidence from dual-task performance indicates that speakers prefer not to select simultaneously resp...
Data, stimuli, and analysis scripts from Brehm, L., Taschenberger, L., & Meyer, A. S. (2019). Mental...
Contains fulltext : 72908.pdf (publisher's version ) (Closed access)Controversy ex...
Despite increasing interest on the topic, the extent to which linguistic processing demands attentio...
In 2 experiments participants named pictures of common objects with superimposed distractor words. I...
Typically, when two individuals perform a task together, each partner monitors the other partners' r...
Contains fulltext : 99699.pdf (publisher's version ) (Closed access) ...
Co-acting participants represent and integrate each other's actions, even when they are not required...
The understanding of the relationship between attention and normal language processing can provide i...
Interference in picture naming occurs from representing a partner's preparations to speak (Gambi, va...
In 4 experiments we showed that picture naming latencies are affected by beliefs about the task conc...
Contains fulltext : 143241pre.pdf (preprint version ) (Open Access)Previous dual-t...
Contains fulltext : 140314pre.pdf (preprint version ) (Open Access)Evidence from d...
Previous dual-task studies examining the locus of semantic interference of distractor words in pictu...
Disagreement exists regarding the functional locus of semantic interference of distractor words in p...
Evidence from dual-task performance indicates that speakers prefer not to select simultaneously resp...
Data, stimuli, and analysis scripts from Brehm, L., Taschenberger, L., & Meyer, A. S. (2019). Mental...
Contains fulltext : 72908.pdf (publisher's version ) (Closed access)Controversy ex...
Despite increasing interest on the topic, the extent to which linguistic processing demands attentio...
In 2 experiments participants named pictures of common objects with superimposed distractor words. I...
Typically, when two individuals perform a task together, each partner monitors the other partners' r...
Contains fulltext : 99699.pdf (publisher's version ) (Closed access) ...
Co-acting participants represent and integrate each other's actions, even when they are not required...
The understanding of the relationship between attention and normal language processing can provide i...