Common vampire bats often regurgitate food to roost-mates that fail to feed. The original explanation for this costly helping behaviour invoked both direct and indirect fitness benefits. Several authors have since suggested that food sharing is maintained solely by indirect fitness because non-kin food sharing could have resulted from kin recognition errors, indiscriminate altruism within groups, or harassment. To test these alternatives, we examined predictors of food-sharing decisions under controlled conditions of mixed relatedness and equal familiarity. Over a 2-year period, we individually fasted 20 vampire bats (Desmodus rotundus) and induced food sharing on 48 of 52 days. Surprisingly, donors initiated food sharing more often than re...
<div>Cooperation events from three studies. The first two studies were conducted on mandrills (Mandr...
Evidence for long-term cooperative relationships comes from several social birds and mam-mals. Vampi...
Food sharing offers a clear example of prosocial behaviour, in which one individual's actions benefi...
Common vampire bats often regurgitate food to roost-mates that fail to feed. The original explanatio...
Common vampire bats often regurgitate food to roost-mates that fail to feed. The original explanatio...
Cooperative behaviors exist along a spectrum of cost, from no-risk scenarios of mutual benefit to se...
In an individualized animal society, social bonds can foster cooperation and enhance survival and re...
Regurgitated food sharing among vampire bats is a classic textbook example of reciprocity ("reciproc...
Behavioural reciprocity can be evolutionarily stable1–3. Initial increase in frequency depends, howe...
Cooperative behaviors exist along a spectrum of cost, from no-risk scenarios of mutual benefit to se...
Some nonhuman animals form adaptive long-term cooperative relationships with nonkin that seem analog...
Many bats are extremely social. In some cases, individuals remain together for years or even decades...
Reciprocal altruism is an example of social behaviour that has generated much interest among evoluti...
Food sharing is often evolutionarily puzzling, because the provider’s benefits are not always clear....
<div>Cooperation events from three studies. The first two studies were conducted on mandrills (Mandr...
Evidence for long-term cooperative relationships comes from several social birds and mam-mals. Vampi...
Food sharing offers a clear example of prosocial behaviour, in which one individual's actions benefi...
Common vampire bats often regurgitate food to roost-mates that fail to feed. The original explanatio...
Common vampire bats often regurgitate food to roost-mates that fail to feed. The original explanatio...
Cooperative behaviors exist along a spectrum of cost, from no-risk scenarios of mutual benefit to se...
In an individualized animal society, social bonds can foster cooperation and enhance survival and re...
Regurgitated food sharing among vampire bats is a classic textbook example of reciprocity ("reciproc...
Behavioural reciprocity can be evolutionarily stable1–3. Initial increase in frequency depends, howe...
Cooperative behaviors exist along a spectrum of cost, from no-risk scenarios of mutual benefit to se...
Some nonhuman animals form adaptive long-term cooperative relationships with nonkin that seem analog...
Many bats are extremely social. In some cases, individuals remain together for years or even decades...
Reciprocal altruism is an example of social behaviour that has generated much interest among evoluti...
Food sharing is often evolutionarily puzzling, because the provider’s benefits are not always clear....
<div>Cooperation events from three studies. The first two studies were conducted on mandrills (Mandr...
Evidence for long-term cooperative relationships comes from several social birds and mam-mals. Vampi...
Food sharing offers a clear example of prosocial behaviour, in which one individual's actions benefi...