Lipreading can evoke an immediate bias on auditory phoneme perception [e.g. 6] and it can produce an aftereffect reflecting a shift in the phoneme boundary caused by exposure to an auditory ambiguous stimulus that is combined with non-ambiguous lipread speech (recalibration, [1]). Here, we tested the stability of lipread-induced recalibration over time. Aftereffects were measured directly after exposure and after 24 hours. Aftereffects dissipated quickly during testing and were not observable anymore after a 24 hours delay
We investigated the effects of adaptation to mouth shapes associated with different spoken sounds (s...
We ordinarily perceive our voice sound as occurring simultaneously with vocal production, but the se...
BACKGROUND: We ordinarily perceive our voice sound as occurring simultaneously with vocal production...
Listeners hearing an ambiguous speech sound flexibly adjust their phonetic categories in accordance ...
Recently, we have shown that lipread speech can recalibrate auditory speech identification when ther...
Although the default state of the world is that we see and hear other people talking, there is evide...
Listeners quickly learn to label an ambiguous speech sound if there is lipread information that tell...
Listeners can flexibly adjust boundaries between phonemes when exposed to biased information. Ambigu...
Although the default state of the world is that we see and hear other people talking, there is evide...
When listeners experience difficulty in understanding a speaker, lexical and audiovisual (or lipread...
Three experiments investigated irrelevant sound interference of lip-read lists. In Experiment 1, an ...
Although the default state of the world is that we see and hear other people talking, there is evide...
When listening to distorted speech, does one become a better listener by looking at the face of the ...
Listeners compensate for coarticulatory influences of one speech sound on another. We examined wheth...
We ordinarily perceive our voice sound as occurring simultaneously with vocal production, but the se...
We investigated the effects of adaptation to mouth shapes associated with different spoken sounds (s...
We ordinarily perceive our voice sound as occurring simultaneously with vocal production, but the se...
BACKGROUND: We ordinarily perceive our voice sound as occurring simultaneously with vocal production...
Listeners hearing an ambiguous speech sound flexibly adjust their phonetic categories in accordance ...
Recently, we have shown that lipread speech can recalibrate auditory speech identification when ther...
Although the default state of the world is that we see and hear other people talking, there is evide...
Listeners quickly learn to label an ambiguous speech sound if there is lipread information that tell...
Listeners can flexibly adjust boundaries between phonemes when exposed to biased information. Ambigu...
Although the default state of the world is that we see and hear other people talking, there is evide...
When listeners experience difficulty in understanding a speaker, lexical and audiovisual (or lipread...
Three experiments investigated irrelevant sound interference of lip-read lists. In Experiment 1, an ...
Although the default state of the world is that we see and hear other people talking, there is evide...
When listening to distorted speech, does one become a better listener by looking at the face of the ...
Listeners compensate for coarticulatory influences of one speech sound on another. We examined wheth...
We ordinarily perceive our voice sound as occurring simultaneously with vocal production, but the se...
We investigated the effects of adaptation to mouth shapes associated with different spoken sounds (s...
We ordinarily perceive our voice sound as occurring simultaneously with vocal production, but the se...
BACKGROUND: We ordinarily perceive our voice sound as occurring simultaneously with vocal production...